Publications
Peripheral targets attenuate miniature eye movements during fixation. Nature Scientific Reports, 23(13), 7426. http://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-34066-2
. (2023). A covered eye fails to follow an object moving in depth. Scientific Reports, 11. http://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-90371-8
. (2021). A common mechanism modulates saccade timing during pursuit and fixation. Journal Of Neurophysiology .
. (2019). Choosing a foveal goal recruits the saccadic system during smooth pursuit. Journal Of Neurophysiology , 120(2), 489-496. http://doi.org/ 10.1152/jn.00418.2017
. (2018). Illusory motion reveals velocity matching, not foveation, drives smooth pursuit of large objects. Journal Of Vision, 17(12).
. (2017). A Subconscious Interaction between Fixation and Anticipatory Pursuit. Journal Of Neuroscience, 37(47), 11424-11430. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2186-17.2017
. (2017). Allocation of attention during pursuit of large objects is no different than during fixation. Journal Of Vision, 15, 9–9.
. (2015). Different time scales of motion integration for anticipatory smooth pursuit and perceptual adaptation. Journal Of Vision, 15, 16.
. (2015). A foveal target increases catch-up saccade frequency during smooth pursuit. Journal Of Neurophysiology, jn–00774.
. (2015). Motion Integration for Ocular Pursuit Does Not Hinder Perceptual Segregation of Moving Objects. The Journal Of Neuroscience, 34, 5835–5841.
. (2014). . (2013).
. (2011).
. (2007).
Perceptual and oculomotor evidence of limitations on processing accelerating motion. Journal Of Vision, 3, 698-709.
. (2003). . (1999).
Local motion detectors cannot account for the detectability of an extended trajectory in noise. Vision Research, 39, 19-30.
. (1999). . (1998).